Amyloid Toxicity: Myths, Contradictions & Failures
For over a
hundred years, since Alois Alzheimer published his famous plaque and tangle
pictures, it has been taken for granted that these plaques and tangles are
actively killing the neurons by being toxic. There was no proof needed of why
or how they are toxic, the details and mechanisms were to come eventually over
time to prove what we already knew all along, these structures are toxic.
Observations and experiments were not performed to test the toxic gain-of-function (GOF) theory of amyloid pathology, but just to verify what we already
know, they are toxic.
Here, and
exactly here, is where the pathology (as a scientific discipline) got its own
disease; confirmation bias. No amount of counterevidence was ever enough to
challenge GOF. Any counterevidence was either accommodated somehow or simply
dismissed, leading to a group of myths and contradictory arguments that are
recited almost religiously in papers, reviews, conferences, and media
interactions to defend GOF. A smokescreen of decisive claims that carefully
hides the weaknesses of GOF and buries any serious challenges to its claims in
obscurity. However, while this was successful for GOF dominance over the field,
it never led to a successful treatment (and this includes aducanumab), because
as Richard Feynman once said, "For a successful technology, reality
must take precedence over public relations, for Nature cannot be fooled."
Here, I will
discuss the major myths and contradictory arguments that are usually weaved
together in acrobatic and confusing statements to give us the epistemological mess
of a theory that is GOF. But first, I will discuss why well-meaning scientists stick
by a clearly weak and ineffective theory. Why??
Why the GOF?
The answer is
one word: genetics. It is the firm belief that there is no other way but GOF to
interpret the genetic evidence that links familial forms of neurodegenerative
diseases to the proteins and peptides found in the amyloid plaques. In the
absence of a better alternative, GOF proponents assert, this is the best we have got,
and we must keep trying intellectually and experimentally to make sense of it
until we succeed. Sounds reasonable, only if it were true, but it is not. The
root conviction that genetics must mean GOF is simply wrong. The genetic
evidence clearly indicates the causal involvement of these proteins in the respective diseases, but it does not indicate how they are involved. Genetics points out
the cause, not the mechanism, and GOF is not the only mechanism by which these
proteins can cause harm.
When a protein aggregates, two things happen simultaneously (like in an hourglass, Fig. 1); as the aggregated species accumulate (GOF), the soluble species gets depleted, and its function is lost (loss-of-function - LOF). The genetic evidence is essentially agnostic and neutral towards the question of whether GOF or LOF is the pathogenic mechanism. So why? Why LOF is ignored despite being equally compatible with the genetic evidence, while GOF gets diehard support? The answer lies in a group of myths that are repeated without actual questioning or validation to dismiss LOF out of hand without any serious consideration. These myths include the following.
Figure 1. Protein aggregation and soluble protein depletion happen simultaneously. |
1. The “no
phenotype” argument: Knock out/down animals of the amyloidogenic proteins show
no phenotype
This is just a blatant fallacy. However, it is usually the first
argument that is used to dismiss LOF as irrelevant and inconsequential. While
it is easily provable to be just a myth, it also flies in the face of common
sense by assuming that the tens of proteins that are involved in amyloid
pathologies, which are evolutionary conserved and produced normally throughout
life, are without function and their depletion has no consequence whatsoever.
Moreover, there are literally tens of papers showing the devastating effects of
knocking out/down Aβ, Tau, α-synuclein, PrPC, SOD-1,
TDP-43, and other proteins that are involved in neurodegenerative diseases. We
cite many of these papers in our review (1 , Box 1) and they are
cited in several other reviews as well (2–4). The “no
phenotype argument” does not make biological sense in the first place, besides
being demonstrably false.
2. OK, they have functions, but we don’t “fully understand” their
functions
Well, we don’t
“fully understand” anything, and science never claims that it will ever do. We
can only understand things to a certain extent and increase our understanding incrementally
over time. We don’t “fully understand” why the corona virus affects certain
people differently and we don’t “fully understand” all the causes of diabetes,
but this still hasn’t paralyzed us from developing successful vaccines and replacement
therapies based on what we know in virology and endocrinology. Moreover, we
don't “fully understand” the mechanisms of toxicity (if any – (see below) ) of
amyloids, but we still spend billions developing drugs targeting them. So, the
mythical and elusive ideal of “fully understanding” is applied selectively to
LOF but not GOF.
Additionally, there
is plenty of literature demonstrating the important functions Aβ and α-synuclein
for example (for Aβ roles in neurogenesis: 10 refs here and synaptic plasticity:
10 refs here, for α-synuclein check ref. (3)). Despite all this knowledge, there is no point
that can be reached where we can claim that we “fully understand” their
functions. However, if we compare LOF and GOF to a more realistic and rational standard
of how they relatively compare to each other in terms of what we
know about both, LOF easily wins. In addition to what we know about the
important functions of these proteins in neurogenesis and synaptic plasticity
for example, LOF is more compatible with the rest of our biological knowledge. Evolutionary
conserved and highly expressed proteins (such as Aβ and α-synuclein) are supposed
to have important functions, and protein depletion, if not compensated, is
never good nor inconsequential. GOF on the other hand struggles with its basic
concepts and mechanisms, and fails to answer simple questions like: what is the
toxic species? What are the physicochemical mechanisms of toxicity or prion “protein-only
replication”? Thus, in realistic standards, we understand much more about
protein function and the detrimental effects of protein depletion than the
hypothetical GOF species and mechanisms.
3. Patients, especially with mutations, have high toxic protein
levels, don’t they?
This is probably the biggest myth of all, entirely false, yet very common. Clinical evidence consistently shows that the soluble levels of the proteins involved in neurodegenerative diseases are LOWER in patients compared to controls. Patients with familial forms of Alzheimer’s disease, both due to mutations (5) or gene duplication (Down syndrome (6)), have lower levels of Aβ42 in the CSF. The same is true for familial forms of Parkinson’s disease and Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease, where the levels α-synuclein and PrPC are lower in patients (7, 8). This is also the case in the sporadic forms of Alzheimer's, Parkinson's, and Creutzfeldt-Jakob diseases (9–11). You expect a toxic protein to be higher in patients, but the opposite is always the case. In fact, a lower soluble level of the protein is a much better predictor of disease status and progression than the amount of aggregates (12). Additionally, Aβ42 depletion is associated with cognitive impairment and dementia in many neurodegenerative diseases that do not specifically harbor Aβ amyloid plaques. These diseases include: frontotemporal dementia, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, Parkinson’s disease, Parkinson’s disease dementia, dementia with Lewy bodies, multiple system atrophy, progressive supranuclear palsy, and corticobasal degeneration (13). Moreover, we have recently found that high levels of soluble Aβ42 in the brain are associated with normal cognition in plaque-positive individuals, no matter how high the plaque burden is in their brains (14) (Fig. 2).
Figure. 2 from Sturchio et al 2021, showing that high levels of soluble Aβ42 preserve cognition regardless of the amyloid plaque burden. |
The abovementioned myths are examples of the easily demonstrably false statements that are used to defend GOF at the expense of LOF. However, GOF is not only inconsistent with the evidence, it is also inconsistent with itself. This can be demonstrated by how contradictory and confusing some of GOF’s basic assumptions are, such as:
1. The most basic GOF premise is that the plaques are the hallmarks of disease, but are they? 25-35% percent of cognitively normal elderly have amyloid plaques in their brains (12). How can something be called “hallmark” if it cannot distinguish between healthy and affected individuals in a quarter to a third of the cases? It is like finding an Eiffel tower in every third or fourth city you visit. At some point, it seizes to be considered a landmark of Paris.
2. GOF was supposed to be based on the toxicity of the hallmark (which is not quite a hallmark), but is it toxic? According to the founders of the two major GOF hypotheses (the amyloid cascade hypothesis and the prion hypothesis), plaques are not toxic, but might actually be protective (Fig. 3).
Figure. 3 |
3. So, a non-hallmark-hallmark that was supposed to be toxic turned out to be protective, probably. Why are the GOF proponents still advising the industry to target the non-toxic, probably-protective plaques?
4. Also, if the plaques that are composed of amyloid fibrils are not toxic, even protective, why is the shape/polymorph/strain of amyloid fibrils important in the pathology if it is not pathological to start with? How would a particular polymorph/strain of fibrils be important in propagating a particular type of pathology if it is protective and non-toxic?
5. If toxicity is not mediated by the non-hallmark-hallmark that turned out to be protective and not toxic, but mediated by an elusive soluble substrate that is called oligomers, how come the patients have LOWER levels of the soluble proteins, the fuel of the supposedly-toxic soluble oligomers?
These are not
open scientific questions; these are blatant contradictions and epic epistemological failures
that should not be present in any theory that claims to be scientific. Taken
together with the demonstrably false myths that are used to keep it going (see
above), GOF is just a terribly bad theory that is neither consistent with
available evidence nor with itself. Moreover, GOF’s mechanismless and
contradictory assumptions are not even consistent with basic physicochemical
and biological principles. In light of established physicochemical principles,
amyloids are too thermodynamically stable (non-reactive) to be toxic
themselves, and oligomers are too thermodynamically transient to be relevant.
Furthermore, there is no physicochemical basis for the so-called “protein-only
replication”, a process that can otherwise be fully explained by the
well-established principles of phase-transition and nucleation (see review (1) for
more details). In terms of established biological principles, proteins evolve to
perform a function, not to be toxic, and uncompensated depletion of soluble
functional proteins is never harmless. Thus, the powerful GOF emperor that has ruled the field for decades is
naked of any internal or external consistency. The only thing that the GOF
theory is consistent at is its failure to produce successful therapies, which
is of course a very natural result given how weak and contradictory it is. Problem-solving is a function of the conceptual framework we use to solve them.
Better
alternative?
There are many
things that are fundamentally wrong about GOF, and it is time to run some
epistemological clean-up and start over. We can start from the points we agree
on, which are:
1. Genetics
says proteins such as Aβ are linked to the pathology
2. Plaques are
not toxic
3. The soluble levels of these proteins are lower in patients compared to controls
4. Proteins
have functions and protein depletion is never inconsequential
Then LOF emerges as a logical necessity, and not a crazy or controversial idea. It is in fact the only way to save the genetic evidence from the convoluted, contradictory, and confusing argumentation of GOF. LOF is compatible with well-established concepts in physical chemistry and biology, and with data from animal models and clinical studies. The only thing remaining to support LOF even further is to actively deplete the soluble levels of the protein in the brain of healthy human subjects and see that it induces cognitive impairment, but who would do such an experiment?!
Unfortunately,
this experiment has already been done, justified by GOF that depleting a functional
protein in healthy human subjects is a good idea. The “successful” depletion of Aβ led to a smaller
brain volume and worse cognition in the treated group compared to the placebo
group (15, 16) (Fig. 4). Many
other clinical trials targeting Aβ have led to a similar outcome, worse
cognition, and more atrophy in the treatment group (17).
Figure 4. from Egan et. al 2019, showing that pharmacological depletion of soluble Aβ induces cognitive impairment in the treatment group compared to placebo. |
Why so harsh?
After the
aducanumab scandal that threatens to bankrupt the healthcare system for a
GOF-based drug that does not work, it is now more important than ever to be
uncompromisingly clear and vocal about the root causes of failure. Science is
not always about finding a middle ground if the data clearly points otherwise.
We cannot keep coming with excuses for a clearly failed research program at the
expense of patients’ time and money. Also, we cannot only blame Biogen for
selecting data while the field has been mired in selectivity, myths, and
contradictions for so long. The field is dominated by a singular
unshakable assumption: amyloids are directly responsible for neurodegeneration via
a GOF toxicity mechanism, and protein depletion, while present, is completely
and absolutely inconsequential and irrelevant. This unitary position is not
only dogmatic and irrational, it is also weak and inconsistent with many
biological and clinical facts. To hide that, GOF was protected by a group of
myths and confusing self-contradictory arguments that kept it alive for so long
and kept any serious challenges to its authority at bay. Aducanumab approval is just the bitter fruit
of a bad tree.
It is time to end GOF
monopoly and expose its flaws with the clearest and strongest terms, since it
has now escaped the realm of academic discussion and poses real-life harm to the
health and finances of millions of people. We need to be clear that the genetic evidence
should not be used to extort people into accepting GOF, since LOF is equally
compatible with the genetic evidence. Additionally, claims such as “knock-out
animals do not have phenotype”, “we don’t fully understand the function”,
and “patients have higher levels of the protein” are factually incorrect
(myths), and should be called out as BS whenever mentioned. Moreover, we
understand much more about protein function than about toxicity mechanisms, and
the hallmark of the disease is protein depletion, irrespective of the plaque
load.
LOF is not a perfect
theory, no scientific theory is, but it is a far more compelling and consistent
theory than GOF. It follows logically and naturally from what we know about physical
chemistry, thermodynamics, evolutionary biology, and protein function and it is compatible
with much of the animal and clinical evidence. It also offers a new way to think
about the pathogenesis and novel path to develop therapeutics based on
non-aggregating protein analogues as replacement therapy to restore the
normal levels and functions of the depleted proteins. The replacement approach already works for
diabetes using insulin and an amylin analogue (pramlintide), why not for
neurodegenerative diseases?
GOF has managed to
push itself to the forefront of research based on a group of contradictory and
mythical assumptions. Now, it is pushing itself even further, from journal pages
and conference halls into patients’ veins and pockets. This requires more than
just rage against aducanumab, it requires exposing the whole research program
for what it is, a total mess, and requires fighting for a better alternative. It
requires a whole new LOFolution!
1. Malmberg,M., Malm,T., Gustafsson,O.,
Wright,A., Andaloussi,S. El and Ezzat,K. (2020) Disentangling the Amyloid
Pathways : A Mechanistic Approach to Etiology. Front. Neurosci.,
14, 1–11.
2. Kent,S.A., Spires-Jones,T.L. and
Durrant,C.S. (2020) The physiological roles of tau and Aβ: implications for
Alzheimer’s disease pathology and therapeutics Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
3. Benskey,M.J., Perez,R.G. and
Manfredsson,F.P. (2016) The contribution of alpha synuclein to neuronal
survival and function - Implications for Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurochem.,
137, 331–359.
4. Saccon,R.A., Bunton-Stasyshyn,R.K.A.,
Fisher,E.M.C. and Fratta,P. (2013) Is SOD1 loss of function involved in
amyotrophic lateral sclerosis? Brain, 136, 2342–2358.
5. Bateman,R.J., Xiong,C.,
Benzinger,T.L.S., Fagan,A.M., Goate,A., Fox,N.C., Marcus,D.S., Cairns,N.J.,
Xie,X., Blazey,T.M., et al. (2012) Clinical and biomarker changes in
dominantly inherited Alzheimer’s disease. N. Engl. J. Med., 367,
795–804.
6. Portelius,E., Hölttä,M., Soininen,H.,
Bjerke,M., Zetterberg,H., Westerlund,A., Herukka,S.K., Blennow,K. and
Mattsson,N. (2014) Altered cerebrospinal fluid levels of amyloid β and amyloid
precursor-like protein 1 peptides in Down’s syndrome. NeuroMolecular Med.,
16, 510–516.
7. Kasuga,K., Tokutake,T., Ishikawa,A.,
Uchiyama,T., Tokuda,T., Onodera,O., Nishizawa,M. and Ikeuchi,T. (2010)
Differential levels of α-synuclein, β-amyloid42 and tau in CSF between patients
with dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol.
Neurosurg. Psychiatry, 81, 608–610.
8. Dorey,A., Tholance,Y., Vighetto,A.,
Perret-Liaudet,A., Lachman,I., Krolak-Salmon,P., Wagner,U., Struyfs,H., De
Deyn,P.P., El-Moualij,B., et al. (2015) Association of cerebrospinal
fluid prion protein levels and the distinction between Alzheimer disease and
Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease. JAMA Neurol., 72, 267–275.
9. Meyne,F., Gloeckner,S.F.,
Ciesielczyk,B., Heinemann,U., Krasnianski,A., Meissner,B. and Zerr,I. (2009)
Total prion protein levels in the cerebrospinal fluid are reduced in patients
with various neurological disorders. J. Alzheimer’s Dis., 17, 863–873.
10. Stomrud,E., Minthon,L., Zetterberg,H.,
Blennow,K. and Hansson,O. (2015) Longitudinal cerebrospinal fluid biomarker
measurements in preclinical sporadic Alzheimer ’ s disease : A
prospective 9-year study. 1, 403–411.
11. Parnetti,L., Paciotti,S., Farotti,L.,
Bellomo,G., Sepe,F.N. and Eusebi,P. (2019) Parkinson’s and Lewy body dementia
CSF biomarkers. Clin. Chim. Acta, 495, 318–325.
12. Villemagne,V.L. and Chételat,G. (2016)
Neuroimaging biomarkers in Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias. Ageing
Res. Rev., 30, 4–16.
13. Mollenhauer,B., Parnetti,L.,
Rektorova,I., Kramberger,M.G., Pikkarainen,M., Schulz-Schaeffer,W.J.,
Aarsland,D., Svenningsson,P., Farotti,L., Verbeek,M.M., et al. (2016)
Biological confounders for the values of cerebrospinal fluid proteins in
Parkinson’s disease and related disorders. J. Neurochem., 139,
290–317.
14. Sturchio,A., Dwivedi,A.K., Young,C.B.,
Malm,T., Marsili,L., Sharma,J.S., Mahajan,A., Hill,E.J., Andaloussi,S. EL,
Poston,K.L., et al. (2021) High cerebrospinal amyloid-β 42 is associated
with normal cognition in individuals with brain amyloidosis. EClinicalMedicine,
000, 100988.
15. Egan,M.F., Kost,J., Voss,T., Mukai,Y.,
Aisen,P.S., Cummings,J.L., Tariot,P.N., Vellas,B., Van Dyck,C.H., Boada,M., et
al. (2019) Randomized trial of verubecestat for prodromal Alzheimer’s
disease. N. Engl. J. Med., 380, 1408–1420.
16. Sur,C., Kost,J., Scott,D., Adamczuk,K.,
Fox,N.C., Cummings,J.L., Tariot,P.N., Aisen,P.S., Vellas,B., Voss,T., et al.
(2020) BACE inhibition causes rapid, regional, and non-progressive volume
reduction in Alzheimer’s disease brain. Brain, 10.1093/brain/awaa332.
17. Espay,A.J., Sturchio,A., Schneider,L.S.
and Ezzat,K. (2021) Soluble Amyloid-β Consumption in Alzheimer’s Disease. J.
Alzheimer’s Dis., 10.3233/jad-210415.
Comments
Post a Comment